AGRUSTIC SOMNACUNI || ROMANY || CRADLE || LET US PRAISE THE ROM || CHUPPA || MEDIATHECA 'FIORETTA MAZZEI' ||  'ENGLISH' CEMETERY || AUREO ANELLO || 2012-2020The Lancet, Early Online Publication, 23 September 2011

INEQUALITY IN EARLY CHILDHOOD:

RISK AND PROTECTIVE FACTORS FOR EARLY CHILD DEVELOPMENT

Summary

Inequality between and within populations has origins in adverse early experiences. Developmental neuroscience shows how early biological and psychosocial experiences affect brain development. We previously identified inadequate cognitive stimulation, stunting, iodine deficiency, and iron-deficiency anaemia as key risks that prevent millions of young children from attaining their developmental potential. Recent research emphasises the importance of these risks, strengthens the evidence for other risk factors including intrauterine growth restriction, malaria, lead exposure, HIV infection, maternal depression, institutionalisation, and exposure to societal violence, and identifies protective factors such as breastfeeding and maternal education. Evidence on risks resulting from prenatal maternal nutrition, maternal stress, and families affected with HIV is emerging. Interventions are urgently needed to reduce children's risk exposure and to promote development in affected children. Our goal is to provide information to help the setting of priorities for early child development programmes and policies to benefit the world's poorest children and reduce persistent inequalities.
This is the first in a Series of two reports about child development

Introduction

In a 2007 Series in The Lancet we estimated that more than 200 million children younger than 5 years from low-income and middle-income countries were not attaining their developmental potential, primarily because of poverty, nutritional deficiencies, and inadequate learning opportunities.1, 2 Economic recession and climate change will probably increase the number of children affected.3, 4 Biological and psychosocial risk factors associated with poverty lead to inequalities in early child development, which undermine educational attainment and adult productivity, thereby perpetuating the poverty cycle.5 In this Series, we review new evidence on the mechanisms and causes of developmental inequality and economic implications and strategies to promote early child development. In this report we summarise evidence from developmental neuroscience on how experiences in early life affect the structure and functioning of the brain, and subsequent child development. We review evidence on risks and protective factors for development, updating evidence on previously identified risks (panel 1),1 and highlight risks not previously identified. Our focus is on modifiable risks that affect large numbers of children younger than 5 years in low-income and middle-income countries.
Panel 1
Previously identified priority risk factors
  • Key risks: inadequate cognitive stimulation, linear growth retardation (stunting), iodine deficiency, and iron-deficiency anaemia
  • Other priority risks: intrauterine growth restriction, malaria, lead exposure, maternal depressive symptoms, and exposure to violence
Key messages
  • Exposure to biological and psychosocial risks affects the developing brain and compromises the development of children
  • Inequalities in child development begin prenatally and in the first years of life
  • With cumulative exposure to developmental risks, disparities widen and trajectories become more firmly established
  • Reducing inequalities requires early integrated interventions that target the many risks to which children in a particular setting are exposed
  • The most effective and cost-efficient time to prevent inequalities is early in life before trajectories have been firmly established
  • Action or lack of action will have lifetime consequences for adult functioning, for the care of the next generation, and for the wellbeing of societies

Risk, stress, and brain development

The foundations of brain architecture are laid down early in life through dynamic interactions of genetic, biological, and psychosocial influences, and child behaviour. Biological and psychosocial influences affect the timing and pattern of genetic expression, which can alter brain structure and function,6 and behaviour.7 Through bidirectional effects, children's behaviour affects brain development directly and by modifying the effects of biological and psychosocial influences.8
Childhood risks associated with poverty, such as lack of stimulation or excessive stress, affect brain development, result in dysregulation of the hypothalamic—pituitary—adrenocortical system,9 and change electrical activity of the brain related to efficiency of cognitive processing.10 The influence of risks can begin prenatally because the fetal brain can be influenced by exogenous factors that produce maternal stress.11 At present there is insufficient evidence from research in human beings to establish if the effects on hypothalamic—pituitary—adrenocortical regulation are reversible.12
Three translational processes influence how risk factors and stress affect brain and behavioural development: the extent and nature of deficits depend on timing, co-occurring and cumulative influences, and differential reactivity (figure 1 and table 1). Risks often co-occur and persist, leading to exposure to multiple and cumulative risks. For example, maternal depression increases risk of low birthweight (LBW; additional references in webappendix pp 1—5), stunting,13 and insecure attachment.14 Because of differential reactivity, the effect of risks on behaviour might vary by individual or environmental characteristics.
Click to toggle image
                            size
Pathways linking poverty to developmental inequities
(A) Timing, dose, and differential reactivity influence how individual exposure to risk and protective factors translate into individual differences in brain function and structure. (B) Brain structure and function influence the degree of differential reactivity shown. (C) Timing and dose of exposure, and differential reactivity moderate the effect of risk and protective factors upon child development.
Click to open
                              table
Translational processes underlying the effect of risk exposure on brain and behavioural development

Maternal nutrition

There is maternal undernutrition (body-mass index <18·5 kg/m2) in 10—19% of women in most low-income and middle-income countries, with higher prevalence in sub-Saharan Africa and south Asia. Maternal pre-pregnancy body-mass index and weight gain during pregnancy predict birthweight, and balanced energy—protein supplementation benefits birthweight and reduces births that are small for their gestational age. However, there is little information on associations between maternal nutritional status and child development. Pre-pregnancy weight and weight gain in Jamaican women that were mostly adequately nourished were not associated with child cognition at age 7 years.15 In Bangladesh, infants of undernourished mothers had poorer problem-solving ability at 7 months,16 and ability was better in infants of mothers given food supplements early rather than later in pregnancy. By age 18 months, no effects of maternal undernutrition or supplementation were identified.17 Analyses of the Dutch (1944—45) and Chinese (1959—61) famines suggest that prenatal nutritional deficits might have long-term effects on adult mental health. There is a need for research on the effect of food supplementation before and during pregnancy on child development.
About 42% of pregnant women in low-income and middle-income countries are anaemic, and, of these, 60% are iron deficient; however, there is little information on perinatal iron deficiency and child development. Lower maternal haemoglobin and neonatal ferritin predicted lower intra-individual variability in temperament-like behaviours in Peruvian infants that suggested diminished responsiveness.18 In South Africa, maternal iron-deficiency anaemia at 6—10 weeks post partum was associated with lower maternal sensitivity and child responsiveness.19 Although both disorders improved after treatment with iron, infant development was delayed at age 9 months.20
Meta-analyses of 12 randomised controlled trials from low-income and middle-income countries show that supplementation with multiple micronutrients in pregnancy leads to increased birthweight. Trials of supplementation with multiple micronutrients during pregnancy in Bangladesh and in pregnant women in Tanzania infected with HIV suggest small benefits to infants' motor development,16, 21 and to mental development in China,22 compared with iron and folic acid alone. In Peru, zinc supplementation during pregnancy had no effect on children's cognitive, social, or behavioural development at ages 4—5 years.23 In Nepal, children whose mothers received iron and folate during pregnancy had better intelligence quotient (IQ), executive, and motor functioning than the placebo group at ages 7—9 years;24 provision of multiple micronutrients or iron plus folate plus zinc had no benefits, possibly because of zinc inhibition of iron absorption.
Inadequate intakes of ω3 fatty acids (including α-linoleic acid, docosahexaenoic acid [DHA], eicosapentaenoic acid) have been reported in pregnant women in some low-income and middle-income countries. In high-income countries, trials of fish oil, DHA, or DHA and eicosapentaenoic acid showed that infants born to supplemented mothers had improvements in visual acuity,25 attention,26 and aspects of cognitive performance.27 Supplementation with ω3 fatty acids and micronutrients benefited birthweight and length and reduced very early preterm births in Chile. In Mexico, supplementation with ω3 fatty acids benefited birthweight and head size in primigravid women only. Information is needed on possible benefits to infant development.

Infant and child nutrition

In low-income and middle-income countries, 16% of births are LBW with rates as great as 27% in south Asia, most of these births being intrauterine growth restriction (IUGR)-LBW. A Guatemalan study28 showed associations between birth size adjusted for gestational age and development at 6 and 24 months, supporting earlier conclusions that IUGR is associated with early developmental risk.1
Evidence for longer-term effects of IUGR is less consistent. Significant effects of birthweight unadjusted for gestational age were identified on IQ at age 5 years29 and on highest school grade achieved.30 However, contributions of prematurity cannot be estimated. No significant differences were identified between term LBW and normal birthweight children in IQ or parent-reported behaviour at 6 years in Jamaica,31 or at 8 years in Brazil,32 and no difference in self-reported behaviour at 12 years in South Africa.33 By contrast, a large study in Taiwan34 reported significant small deficits in academic achievement of term LBW at 15 years. More evidence is needed on long-term effects of IUGR in low-income and middle-income countries on IQ, and specific cognitive and social skills.
About 39% of infants aged 0—6 months in low-income and middle-income countries are exclusively breastfed, with wide variations in duration of exclusive breastfeeding between countries. In a large cluster-randomised trial in Belarus,35 clinics were assigned to breastfeeding promotion or usual care. Intervention increased exclusive breastfeeding at 3 months and any breastfeeding up to 12 months. At age 6·5 years, intervention children had significantly higher scores on verbal and full-scale IQ and teacher ratings for reading and writing. No benefits were identified for child behaviour.36 In Brazil, boys breastfed for at least 9 months attained 0·5—0·8 school grades more by 18 years than boys breastfed for less than 1 month. Regression of grade level attained on adult income in this population suggests this difference corresponds to a 10—15% difference in income.37 These findings strengthen the evidence for benefits of breastfeeding to development and educational attainment.
In high-income countries, formula-fed infants given DHA supplemented formula had better visual acuity, with greater benefits for preterm infants. There is little information on essential fatty-acid intake or the developmental effect in infants and children from low-income and middle-income countries. In Turkey, improvements in brainstem auditory evoked potentials were noted in infants randomly assigned to receive DHA-supplemented formula compared with infants receiving non-supplemented formula.38 Consumption of complementary foods fortified with micronutrients and essential fatty acids was associated with improved motor development in Ghana and China.39, 40 Although it is unclear which nutrients were responsible for the benefits, supplementation with essential fatty acids and micronutrients resulted in earlier walking compared with micronutrients alone;39 however, the groups also differed in energy intake.
Linear growth retardation or stunting is estimated to affect 34% of children younger than 5 years in low-income and middle-income countries. Consistent with previous evidence, new longitudinal studies from Brazil, India, Peru, and Vietnam show associations between early height-for-age and cognitive or language ability at 5 years.
Height before 6 years was related to age at school enrolment and grades attained by late adolescence in Zimbabwe.41 New information also extends the long-term outcomes associated with stunting, including reduced likelihood of formal employment at age 20—22 years in the Philippines42 and poorer psychological functioning in Jamaican adolescents.43
Timing of growth faltering seems important. In Guatemala, growth and development were related up to age 24 months but not from 24 to 36 months.28 Pooled analyses of five longitudinal studies identified that a 1 SD increase in weight gain from birth to 24 months was associated with increased schooling (0·43 years) and inversely related to grade failures, whereas growth from 2 to 4 years had little affect.30 Duration might also change the effect because Peruvian children stunted at age 6—18 months, but not at 4·5—6 years, did not differ from children who were not stunted at either age in vocabulary and quantitative test scores at 4·5—6 years. Children stunted at both ages had significantly lower scores. The timing of catch-up growth is unknown and might have happened within the first 2 years of life.44
Previous randomised controlled trials of macronutrient supplementation to promote better growth consistently showed concurrent developmental benefits.1 Follow-up of a cluster-randomised trial in Guatemala showed benefits to reading comprehension and reasoning at 25—42 years in participants supplemented from birth to 24 months, but not those supplemented later.45 Men supplemented throughout the first 3 years earned higher hourly wages.46 These findings highlight the importance of adequate nutrition early in life.
Several studies reported previously unrecognised behavioural or neurophysiological alterations with iron-deficiency anaemia in infancy (webappendix pp 9—28). Studies in Chile, India, and Mexico identified electrophysiological evidence of delayed brain maturation in infants with iron-deficiency anaemia. Sleep duration improved with iron plus folic acid or zinc supplementation, but not both, in trials in Zanzibar and Nepal.47 However, sleep-state organisation was altered in Chilean children aged 4 years despite treatment for iron-deficiency anaemia in infancy.48 Additional evidence from studies in Chile, India, Mexico, and Zanzibar showed poorer cognitive, motor, and social—emotional development associated with iron-deficiency anaemia in infancy, or the preschool period. Social—emotional development improved in Chilean infants with iron-deficiency anaemia who received home visitation to promote development, but remained lower than that of non-anaemic infants. Without home visitation social—emotional development declined in infants with iron-deficiency anaemia.49
Costa Rican adolescents who had chronic, severe iron deficiency with or without anaemia in infancy showed no catch-up in motor development despite iron therapy in infancy,50 poorer executive functioning and recognition memory at age 19 years,51 and more internalising and externalising behaviour problems in childhood and adolescence.52 A study of fortification of complementary feeding in China noted infants whose anaemia did not correct within 6 months had lower IQ at age 6 years than those whose anaemia resolved.40
In addition to iron, many other micronutrients are deficient in children in low-income and middle-income countries including zinc, vitamins A, B12, D, E, riboflavin, and iodine in some regions. Six randomised and one non-randomised trial of supplementation with multiple micronutrients or fortification included three or more micronutrients and assessed development in children younger than 5 years (webappendix pp 29—37). Five of seven studies showed benefits to motor development. Studies from Bangladesh and India assessing mental development did not identify any benefits,53, 54 and one from China identified small benefits for mental development at 24 months and for IQ at 6 years.40 There are insufficient data to establish whether supplementation with multiple micronutrients is more effective than iron alone in improving development.

Infectious diseases

Previous evidence of the effect of diarrhoea on child development was inconclusive. Additional studies in Brazil noted associations between the number of diarrhoea episodes before age 2 years, late school entry,55 deficits in semantic fluency, and verbal learning,56 adjusting for socioeconomic status and present nutritional status. Adjustment for stunting before age 2 attenuates the association between diarrhoea and intellectual performance.29 A multicountry study showed that each episode of diarrhoea in the first 2 years of life contributes to stunting,57 suggesting that associations between diarrhoea early in life and school-age performance might be through the same processes that cause stunting.
1·2 billion people are at risk of malaria, with children younger than 5 years at greatest risk. Cerebral or severe malaria can have serious neurological sequelae including seizures, and language and cognitive deficits.1, 58 In Uganda, cognitive training interventions improved the function of affected children.59
New evidence suggests that repeated uncomplicated attacks and asymptomatic parasitaemia (experienced by millions of children annually) also affect children's development. In a cross-country analysis controlling for education quality and other confounders, grade repetition and primary school completion rates were related to malaria exposure.60 Longitudinal studies with school-aged children from Brazil and Mali have shown associations between attacks of clinical malaria or asymptomatic parasitaemia and poorer cognitive scores and academic performance. Randomised clinical trials of chemoprophylaxis in schoolchildren showed significant benefits to language, mathematics, and attendance in Sri Lanka,61 and to attention in Kenya.62
There are fewer studies with children younger than 5 years. A history of malaria attacks was associated with poorer cognitive function at school entry in Sri Lanka,63 and there were inconsistent associations between parasitaemia and activity and exploration in toddlers in Zanzibar.64 Chemoprophylaxis in young children in The Gambia had later benefits for grades attained65 but not cognitive function, although duration of intervention was related to cognitive function. Although most data come from studies of school-aged children, malaria attacks are more common and severe in younger children, and cognitive effects might be worse. Despite progress in control programmes, in 18 African countries surveyed only 23% of children younger than 5 years and 27% of pregnant women were sleeping under insecticide-treated nets.
Most studies investigating other parasitic infections and child cognitive or social—emotional performance involve school-age children. The few studies with young children are inconclusive.1 Although one additional study from Brazil29 showed an association between the number of parasitic infections at 1—3 years and lower IQ at 5 years, findings were not significant after covariate control. Evidence is insufficient to establish if early parasitic infections affect child development.
An estimated 2·1 million children younger than 15 years are living with HIV; however, only 28% of children in low-income and middle-income countries who need antiretroviral drugs receive them. HIV infection affects brain development, leading to cognitive impairments.66 Detrimental effects of HIV infection on neurocognitive development were identified in 36 of 43 studies from low-income, middle-income, and high-income countries.67 We summarise in the webappendix (pp 33—37) studies of the development of children younger than 5 years infected with HIV from low-income and middle-income countries. Compared with uninfected children, children infected with HIV had significantly lower motor and mental development scores in most studies. Effects are accentuated by associated illnesses, poor nutritional status, and adverse living conditions, including caregiver stress, illness, and death (co-occurrence or cumulative influences).
In US studies, highly active antiretroviral therapy (HAART) has led to reduced rates of progressive HIV encephalopathy68 and some benefits to development.69 Cognitive function did not change after short-term treatment (6 months) in South African children;70 however, benefits to motor and cognitive development were noted after 1 year in the Democratic Republic of the Congo with greater benefits in younger children.71 There is an urgent need for increased access to treatment for infected children in low-income and middle-income countries and further assessment of the effect of early treatment on development.
Cognitive and motor deficits have been reported in HIV-exposed uninfected children in low-income and middle-income countries including the Democratic Republic of the Congo72 and Thailand.73 However, co-varying risks such as family poverty and non-parental caregivers were also increased and other studies have not identified deficits (webappendix pp 33—37). Many uninfected children are affected by parental HIV, which can increase exposure to developmental risks such as poverty,74 disrupted caregiving,75 and abandonment.76 In South Africa, young children in affected households with caregiver illness or death were at risk for bullying, mental health problems,77 and abuse,74 and in Rwanda for emotional and behavioural problems.78 The restricted financial and social support available to non-parental caregivers further challenges the wellbeing of orphans.79

Environmental toxins

Children might be exposed to environmental toxins prenatally—through maternal exposure—and postnatally—through breastmilk, food, water, house dust, or soil. We previously identified lead as a risk factor for young children from low-income and middle-income countries.1 Recent evidence from Poland has shown that prenatal exposure to very low concentrations of lead (<5 μg/dL) can result in poor mental development in young children.80
Evidence from low-income and middle-income countries on the effect of other toxins on early child development is inconsistent or sparse (webappendix pp 38—39). Evidence from China shows that arsenic exposure can compromise cognition in older children;81 however, studies from Bangladesh have not identified significant associations between arsenic exposure and mental development up to age 2 years.17 Prenatal exposure to mercury has been linked to low cognitive performance in infancy and early childhood in Brazil,82 but studies from the Seychelles report weak or inconsistent effects,83 or no effects.84 In Ecuador, prenatal exposure to pesticides was significantly associated with poor communication and motor skills;85 however, associations with later development were weaker,85 or non-significant in Mexico.86 Prenatal exposure to polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons was associated with slower language and cognitive development up to age 2 years in China87 and intelligence at age 5 years in Poland.88
Comparison of findings is difficult because of variability in exposure duration, timing, and outcome measures.83 Inconsistent findings might also relate to differential reactivity, in which effects are modified by risk factors, such as low birthweight or malnutrition.85 Alternatively, the effect of toxins might be reduced when exposure is associated with protective influences, such as polyunsaturated fatty acids in mercury contaminated fish, or better health care for children of mothers employed on farms. Further evidence is needed of the effects of toxins on early child development as well as further assessment of interactions with other exposures.

Disabilities

In a survey of disability in 18 low-income and middle-income countries, 23% of children aged 2—9 years had, or were at risk for, disabilities. Besides being a marker for compromised development, childhood disabilities can reduce access to school or health services, and increase risk of caregiver stress and depression89, 90 (webappendix p 40). Studies from south Asia suggest that learning and social integration is also limited by social stigma89 and overprotection by parents.90
Although interventions can promote better function in children with disabilities, few have been assessed in low-income and middle-income countries. Randomised trials suggest more positive attitudes after interactive group therapy in parents of children with intellectual disabilities in India,91 and benefits from mother—child group intervention or parent training to child development and maternal adaptation for children with cerebral palsy in Bangladesh.92 Quasiexperimental studies of parent-training programmes have shown some benefits to child development and maternal behaviour (webappendix p 40).
Evidence on availability of services is scarce but studies from Pakistan and South Africa report that few children receive adequate services.89, 93 Identifying barriers to accessing services is an important priority for children with disabilities. Community-based approaches to provision of services are discussed in the second paper in this Series.

Psychosocial factors

Early learning and caregiver—child interaction

Learning opportunities that facilitate early cognitive development include caregiver activities and materials that promote age-appropriate language and problem-solving skills. Caregiver—child interactions that facilitate early social—emotional development include caregiver positive emotionality, sensitivity, and responsiveness toward the child, and avoidance of harsh physical punishment. Lack of early learning opportunities and appropriate caregiver—child interactions contribute to loss of developmental potential.1 We review new studies that assess the effect of interventions to increase learning opportunities and improve caregiver—child interaction (table 2 and webappendix pp 41—45). The second paper in the Series discusses the effectiveness of interventions that are, or could be, implemented at scale.
Click to open
                                table
Effects of early interventions on cognitive and social—emotional development
Studies from Bangladesh, China, India, and South Africa have shown that interventions to enhance mother—child interactions and increase developmentally facilitative activities benefit cognitive development when delivered through home visits,98 individual parent counselling delivered at health centres,94, 96 or combined approaches.95 Benefits have been shown in children with risk conditions such as severe malnutrition,98 LBW,95 iron-deficiency anaemia,49 or HIV infection.96 Group parenting education benefited mental development in one of three studies (webappendix p 41—45).
In Chile and South Africa, early interventions to improve mother—child interaction promoted attachment14 and social—emotional development,49 although gains were not identified in Bangladesh.98 A preschool intervention in Jamaica to promote social—emotional development reduced child-behaviour problems.97
Sustained intervention benefits to cognitive function at age 18 years have previously been reported.1 Studies from Jamaica and Turkey show benefits to college attendance,99 psychological functioning,100 and cognition and behaviour at age 6 years.31

Maternal depression

A recent study from Bangladesh provides further evidence of the high incidence of maternal depressive symptoms in many low-income and middle-income countries. Maternal depressive symptoms are negatively associated with early child development and quality of parenting across different cultures and socioeconomic groups.101 In Bangladesh, maternal depressive symptoms were associated with infant stunting, perhaps related to unresponsive caregiving13 (webappendix p 46). Risk factors for maternal depression, such as poverty, low education, high stress, lack of empowerment, and poor social support101 are also risk factors for poor child development, suggesting that the relation between maternal depression and compromised early child development is multilevel and cumulative.
Availability of mental health care is restricted in many low-income and middle-income countries. In Pakistan and South Africa, interventions delivered by community health workers have reduced maternal depressive symptoms,12, 102 and improved maternal sensitivity and infant attachment,14 infant health, and time spent playing with infants.102 Evidence that symptoms of maternal depression can be effectively treated in low-income and middle-income countries, often with restricted resources and community health workers, emphasises the need for early identification and community programmes to reduce the risk of adverse consequences for mothers and children.

Exposure to violence

Estimates suggest that 300 million children younger than 5 years have been exposed to societal violence. New studies further show the adverse consequences of exposure to violence in young children (webappendix p 47). Although domestic violence and child abuse happen in countries of all incomes, we focus here on societal or community violence that might be particularly common in low-income and middle-income countries.
Young children exposed to societal violence show insecure attachments,103 increased risk of behaviour problems,104 reduced levels of prosocial behaviour, and increased aggressive behaviour.105 The adverse consequences might result from disruptions to family structure and function106 that compromise the adequacy of maternal childrearing skills,103 and reduce children's ability to regulate their own emotions.105
Studies from Israel and Palestine identified intervention strategies that can reduce stress reactions for young children.107, 108 The effect of exposure to violence can be reduced by supportive parental reactions and positive family routines; however, violence can disrupt the quality of parenting, thereby reducing families' ability to protect young children exposed to violence.107

Institutionalisation

At least 2 million children are institutionalised in non-parental-group residential care. This is probably an underestimate because of under-reporting and lack of information for some regions. Use of orphanages and other institutional care seems to be increasing. Although children's response to institutionalisation varies, many show long-term developmental deficits.109 Institutional rearing starting early in life increases children's risk for adverse outcomes including poor growth, ill-health, attachment disorders, attention disorders, poor cognitive function, anxiety, and autistic-like behaviour109, 110 (webappendix p 48).
Recent studies of institutionalised children show the effect of early experiences on brain development. Institutional rearing has been associated with reduced metabolism in the temporal and frontal cortices, reductions in white-matter connectivity, reductions in brain electrical activity, dysregulation of the hypothalamic—pituitary—adrenocortical system, and changes in brain volume (particularly the amygdala; table 3 and webappendix p 48). Illustrating the translational processes of timing and cumulative exposure (table 1), children experiencing longer institutional placement show larger reductions in left amygdala volume111 and greater dysregulation of the hypothalamic—pituitary—adrenocortical axis,112 whereas children adopted from institutions before the second year of life have more normalised amygdala volume113 and brain electrical activity.114 Adverse neural consequences underlie the behavioural sequelae of early institutionalisation.115
Click to open
                                table
Neural consequences of institutionalisation
Improving the institutional environment (eg, training staff in sensitive responsive caregiving; increasing caregiver stability and the caregiver-to-child ratio) results in significant benefits to child cognitive and social—emotional competence.116 Foster placement and adoption are preferable alternatives to institutionalisation,109, 117 particularly if foster and adoptive families receive adequate support.

Protective influences

Protective factors attenuate adverse consequences of risk factors. Although risk and protective factors are conceptually distinct, many protective factors are the inverse of risk factors (eg, insecure attachment vs secure attachment). Studies in high-income countries have identified biological, psychosocial, and behavioural protective factors for young children, but there are few studies from low-income and middle-income countries. The protective effects of breastfeeding and early cognitive and social—emotional stimulation were reviewed in previous sections. Maternal education also can act as a protective factor, reducing child mortality and promoting early child development (webappendix pp 49—50).
Young children of educated mothers have higher levels of cognitive development than children of less educated mothers.118—120 Similarly, high-risk infants121 and young children122 show better developmental trajectories when their mothers have higher levels of education.
In panel 2 we show the protective mechanisms linking maternal education and early child development. Children of less-educated mothers are likely to have greater exposure to developmental risks and less access to interventions than children of more-educated mothers, suggesting that low maternal education identifies families in need of intervention.118 However, poorly educated women might benefit less from participation in parent-focused programmes than better-educated women124 (differential reactivity), emphasising the need for strategies to increase their participation and learning in early child-development interventions.
Panel 2
Protective mechanisms associated with more maternal education*
Less maternal depression
  • Lower risk of maternal depression and non-depressed mothers provide a more optimum rearing environment for their children
Child nutritional status
  • Infants and young children with better nutritional status
Quality of child-rearing environment
  • Greater knowledge about child development
  • More likely to use developmentally appropriate child-rearing strategies and provide more stimulating home environments
  • Possess a wider variety of child-rearing strategies
  • More sensitive to individual differences in children's developmental trajectories
  • Have higher educational aspirations for their children
Ability to access and benefit from interventions
  • More likely to make use of available intervention services; are more likely to be involved in and comply with intervention programmes
  • Better able to comprehend intervention material (eg, growth charts)
  • Have greater recall of intervention material
References in the webappendix pp 4—5.
* Maternal education is a unique protective factor, even after adjusting for family economics.123

Conclusions

Major advances in neuroscience show how exposure to biological and psychosocial risk factors, prenatally and during early childhood, affects brain structure and function and compromises children's development and subsequent developmental trajectory. We summarise in figure 2 how risk and protective factors encountered before age 5 years compromise children's development. The greater the exposure to cumulative risks the greater the inequality, suggesting that early interventions that prevent inequality are more effective than later interventions, which attempt to remedy cumulative deficits. Risk factors are likely to co-occur, emphasising the importance of integrated interventions involving the simultaneous reduction of multiple risks. The second paper in the Series discusses integrated interventions.
Click to toggle image
                            size
Differing trajectories of brain and behavioural development as a function of exposure to risk and protective factors
The cumulative effect is illustrated by the progressive strengthening (darker lines) of the trajectories over time.
Inequalities in low-income and middle-income countries are established in early childhood and contribute to lifetime differences. Accumulated developmental deficits in early childhood place children on a lower life-time trajectory with negative implications for adult cognitive and psychological functioning, educational attainment, and subsequent income, thus contributing to continued inequalities in the next generation.
In table 4, we list the risk and protective factors with sufficient evidence to be priorities for intervention and summarise the evidence reviewed. Previously identified key risks (inadequate stimulation, stunting, iodine deficiency, iron-deficiency anaemia) remain in need of urgent intervention to prevent the loss of developmental potential in millions of young children. Although there has been recent attention to the effect of early nutrition on development and health,125 substantial progress in improving development is unlikely to be made without also increasing early learning opportunities.126 A meta-analysis of non-US intervention studies127 showed that cognitive benefits were greater when interventions included stimulation or education components compared with those comprising nutrition or economic assistance only. This strengthens the case for integration of stimulation with economic, nutrition, and health interventions.
Click to open
                              table
High priority developmental risk and protective factors
New research strengthens the evidence for prioritisation of interventions to reduce the levels of IUGR, malaria, maternal depression, institutionalisation, and exposure to societal violence and to promote development in affected children. New research also suggests the adverse consequences for children infected with HIV or whose parents are infected. We highlight the importance of protective factors such as breastfeeding and higher maternal education, which can reduce the effect of risks. Knowledge of risk and protective factors can inform priorities for programmes and funding to promote early child development. This knowledge, plus increased understanding of the neural consequences of risks, provides persuasive data for advocacy and the design of early intervention programmes to reduce developmental inequalities.
Although effective interventions exist for some identified risks, further research is needed to increase our ability to promote early child development in low-income and middle-income countries. We list research priorities in panel 3. There has been little progress in some previously identified research priorities (eg, supplementation with multiple micronutrients, prenatal iron deficiency, and exposure to toxins). Additional research questions include the effect of prenatal maternal nutrition and stress on development, assessment of the effect of interventions to reduce maternal depression on child development, and assessment of strategies to reduce the developmental consequences for children affected by violence and for children in families affected by HIV. Research is also needed to develop strategies to include children with disabilities in early child development programmes and provide them with specialist services, and to identify additional protective factors in low-income and middle-income countries.
Panel 3
Priorities for future research to reduce developmental inequalities in infants and young children from low-income and middle-income countries
Maternal nutrition
  • Effect of food supplementation before and during pregnancy on development of infants and young children.
  • Effect of prenatal iron deficiency on postnatal cognitive and social—emotional development.
  • Effect of supplementation with multiple micronutrients in pregnancy on child development by comparison with iron and folic acid alone.
  • Effect of maternal supplementation with ω3 fatty acids on infant development.
  • Long-term effects of IUGR on cognitive and social—emotional outcomes.
Child nutrition
  • Effect of improving infant intake of essential fatty acids on development.
  • Effect of supplementation with multiple micronutrients on development and comparison with effects of iron only.
  • How to integrate nutrition and psychosocial stimulation programmes at scale.
Infections
  • Effect of malaria prevention strategies on early child development.
  • Effect of antiretroviral treatment on cognitive and behavioural outcomes and effect of non-medical interventions to promote development in children infected with HIV.
  • Extent of mental health problems for infants and young children orphaned because of AIDS. Assessment of interventions to support caregivers and promote development of children affected by HIV.
Toxins
  • Evidence on effect of toxins is inconsistent possibly because of interactions with other exposures. Longitudinal studies are needed to assess potential moderating variables (eg, nutrition).
Disabilities
  • Assessment of the effect of interventions for children with disability and their families.
  • Identification of barriers to accessing general services (eg, primary health care) as well as specialist services.
Learning opportunities and stimulation
  • Modification of interventions to facilitate expansion, and assessment of effectiveness of programmes at scale.
  • More evidence on the effect of early interventions on social and emotional development.
Maternal depression
  • Assessment of effect of interventions to reduce depressive symptoms on child development and identification of strategies to expand access.
Violence
  • Evidence needed on the neural and developmental effect of violence exposure on children younger than 5 years and on effective treatment strategies for young children exposed to violence.
Protective factors
  • Need to identify additional protective factors for outcomes related to early child development in low-income and middle-income countries.
Without the threats of biological and psychosocial risks, and with a caregiving environment that supports cognitive and social—emotional development, children experience healthy brain development that enables them to reach toward their developmental potential. With this strong foundation, they build lifespan developmental trajectories that enable them to benefit from family, community, and educational opportunities (figure 2). Effective interventions to promote early child development in low-income and middle-income countries exist either at scale or are potentially scalable. Interventions to reduce risks and support early child development will yield lifetime gains that contribute to the achievement and sustainability of improved development in the next generation. By investing in early child development programmes, we have an opportunity to break the cycle of inequities that has dominated the lives of millions of children and families in low-income and middle-income countries.

Search strategy and selection criteria

We searched relevant databases (eg, PubMed, PsychInfo, Cochrane Review) with multiple search terms for articles published since 2005. The search terms we used were linked to each of the risk or protective factors: “child development”, “child behaviour”, “infant behaviour”, “cognition”, “social”, “emotional”, “intelligence”, “language”, and “motor development”. We searched citation lists of articles retrieved and review articles published since the last Series for further references. We included earlier key publications in which the risk or protective factor was not reviewed in the previous Series. We include only risk and protective factors that can be modified by interventions or public policy and which affect large numbers of children younger than 5 years in low-income and middle-income countries. We consider exposures in utero to age 5 years and focus on research done in low-income and middle-income countries. Although many of the risk and protective factors we considered are also relevant to children's health outcomes, we focus on children's cognitive, motor, and social—emotional development.
Contributors
All authors participated in the review of published work, and drafting and review of the report. SPW and TDW are the lead authors of this report and were responsible for the final draft and the decision to submit for publication. SG-M and MMB provided critical revision of the text. Reviews and drafting of individual topics were as follows: Brain development CAN and TDW; maternal undernutrition SG-M; micronutrients SG-M and MMB; essential fatty acids SLH; IUGR SPW; breastfeeding CAP; stunting SG-M; iron deficiency BL; diarrhoea MMB; malaria SG-M; other parasitic infections TDW; HIV JMM and LR; toxins JDH; disabilities HB-H; early learning opportunities SPW, SMC, and HBH; maternal depression AR; violence JMM and TDW; institutionalisation CAN, SG-M, and LR; and protective factors TDW. The steering committee of the Global Child Development Group coordinated the writing of the report in this Series.
Conflicts of interest
We declare that we have no conflicts of interest.
Acknowledgments
We thank Amika Wright for assistance with referencing and Anna Quigg for assistance with figure 2. A meeting of all authors to discuss review findings and coordinate the report was held in Jamaica in December, 2009, with the support of the Global Alliance for Improved Nutrition (GAIN), UNICEF, the Bernard van Leer Foundation, and the University of the West Indies. A follow-up steering committee meeting was held in May, 2010, with the support of UNICEF, the Bernard van Leer Foundation, and the Child Health and Nutrition Research Initiative. The sponsors had no role in the design and conduct of the review, interpretation and writing or the decision to submit for publication. HBH was supported by a Wellcome Trust Fellowship (# 080534/Z/06/Z). We thank the Global Child Development Group Secretariat for coordinating the meetings.

WebExtra Content

Supplementary webappendix
Open file
PDF (766K)

References

1 Walker SP, Wachs TD, Gardner JM, et al. Child development: risk factors for adverse outcomes in developing countries. Lancet 2007; 369: 145-157. Summary | Full Text | PDF(246KB) | CrossRef | PubMed
2 Grantham-McGregor S, Cheung YB, Cueto S, Glewwe P, Richter L, Strupp B. Developmental potential in the first 5 years for children in developing countries. Lancet 2007; 369: 60-70. Summary | Full Text | PDF(552KB) | CrossRef | PubMed
3 Friedman J, Sturdy J. The influence of economic crisis on early childhood development: a review of pathways and measured impact. In: Alderman H, ed. No small matter: the impact of poverty, shocks and human capital investments in early childhood development. Washington, DC: The World Bank, 2011: 51-83.
4 Sheffield P, Landrigan P. Global climate change and children's health: threats and strategies for prevention. Environ Health Perspect 2011; 119: 291-298. CrossRef | PubMed
5 Engle PL, Black MM, Behrman JR, et al. Strategies to avoid the loss of developmental potential in more than 200 million children in the developing world. Lancet 2007; 369: 229-242. Summary | Full Text | PDF(292KB) | CrossRef | PubMed
6 Hertzman C, Boyce T. How experience gets under the skin to create gradients in developmental health. Annu Rev Public Health 2010; 31: 329-347. CrossRef | PubMed
7 Dawson G, Ashman SB, Carver LJ. The role of early experience in shaping behavioral and brain development and its implications for social policy. Dev Psychopathol 2000; 12: 695-712. CrossRef | PubMed
8 Stiles J. The fundamentals of brain development: integrating nature and nurture. Cambridge, MA: Harvard University Press, 2008.
9 Fernald LC, Gunnar MR. Poverty-alleviation program participation and salivary cortisol in very low-income children. Soc Sci Med 2009; 68: 2180-2189. CrossRef | PubMed
10 Hackman DA, Farah MJ. Socioeconomic status and the developing brain. Trends Cogn Sci 2009; 13: 65-73. CrossRef | PubMed
11 Talge NM, Neal C, Glover V. Antenatal maternal stress and long-term effects on child neurodevelopment: how and why?. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2007; 48: 245-261. CrossRef | PubMed
12 Dowd JB, Simanek AM, Aiello AE. Socio-economic status, cortisol and allostatic load: a review of the literature. Int J Epidemiol 2009; 38: 1297-1309. CrossRef | PubMed
13 Black MM, Baqui AH, Zaman K, El Arifeen S, Black RE. Maternal depressive symptoms and infant growth in rural Bangladesh. Am J Clin Nutr 2009; 89: 951S-957S. CrossRef | PubMed
14 Cooper PJ, Tomlinson M, Swartz L, et al. Improving quality of mother-infant relationship and infant attachment in socioeconomically deprived community in South Africa: randomised controlled trial. BMJ 2009; 338: b974. CrossRef | PubMed
15 Walker SP, Thame MM, Chang SM, Bennett F, Forrester TE. Association of growth in utero with cognitive function at age 6—8 years. Early Hum Dev 2007; 83: 355-360. CrossRef | PubMed
16 Tofail F, Persson LA, El Arifeen S, et al. Effects of prenatal food and micronutrient supplementation on infant development: a randomized trial from the Maternal and Infant Nutrition Interventions, Matlab (MINIMat) study. Am J Clin Nutr 2008; 87: 704-711. PubMed
17 Hamadani JD, Grantham-McGregor SM, Tofail F, et al. Pre- and postnatal arsenic exposure and child development at 18 months of age: a cohort study in rural Bangladesh. Int J Epidemiol 2010; 39: 1206-1216. CrossRef | PubMed
18 Wachs TD, Kanashiro HC, Gurkas P. Intra-individual variability in infancy: structure, stability, and nutritional correlates. Dev Psychobiol 2008; 50: 217-231. CrossRef | PubMed
19 Murray-Kolb LE, Beard JL. Iron deficiency and child and maternal health. Am J Clin Nutr 2009; 89: 946S-950S. CrossRef | PubMed
20 Perez EM, Hendricks MK, Beard JL, et al. Mother-infant interactions and infant development are altered by maternal iron deficiency anemia. J Nutr 2005; 135: 850-855. PubMed
21 McGrath N, Bellinger D, Robins J, Msamanga GI, Tronick E, Fawzi WW. Effect of maternal multivitamin supplementation on the mental and psychomotor development of children who are born to HIV-1-infected mothers in Tanzania. Pediatrics 2006; 117: e216-e225. PubMed
22 Li Q, Yan H, Zeng L, et al. Effects of maternal multimicronutrient supplementation on the mental development of infants in rural western China: follow-up evaluation of a double-blind, randomized, controlled trial. Pediatrics 2009; 123: e685-e692. CrossRef | PubMed
23 Caulfield LE, Putnick DL, Zavaleta N, et al. Maternal gestational zinc supplementation does not influence multiple aspects of child development at 54 mo of age in Peru. Am J Clin Nutr 2010; 92: 130-136. CrossRef | PubMed
24 Christian P, Murray-Kolb LE, Khatry SK, et al. Prenatal micronutrient supplementation and intellectual and motor function in early school-aged children in Nepal. JAMA 2010; 304: 2716-2723. CrossRef | PubMed
25 Innis SM, Friesen RW. Essential n-3 fatty acids in pregnant women and early visual acuity maturation in term infants. Am J Clin Nutr 2008; 87: 548-557. PubMed
26 Colombo J, Kannass KN, Shaddy DJ, et al. Maternal DHA and the development of attention in infancy and toddlerhood. Child Dev 2004; 75: 1254-1267. PubMed
27 Judge MP, Harel O, Lammi-Keefe CJ. Maternal consumption of a docosahexaenoic acid-containing functional food during pregnancy: benefit for infant performance on problem-solving but not on recognition memory tasks at age 9 mo. Am J Clin Nutr 2007; 85: 1572-1577. PubMed
28 Kuklina EV, Ramakrishnan U, Stein AD, Barnhart HH, Martorell R. Early childhood growth and development in rural Guatemala. Early Hum Dev 2006; 82: 425-433. CrossRef | PubMed
29 Santos DN, Assis AM, Bastos AC, et al. Determinants of cognitive function in childhood: a cohort study in a middle income context. BMC Public Health 2008; 8: 202. CrossRef | PubMed
30 Martorell R, Horta BL, Adair LS, et al. Weight gain in the first two years of life is an important predictor of schooling outcomes in pooled analyses from five birth cohorts from low- and middle-income countries. J Nutr 2010; 140: 348-354. CrossRef | PubMed
31 Walker SP, Chang SM, Younger N, Grantham-McGregor SM. The effect of psychosocial stimulation on cognition and behaviour at 6 years in a cohort of term, low-birthweight Jamaican children. Dev Med Child Neurol 2010; 52: e148-e154. CrossRef | PubMed
32 Emond AM, Lira PI, Lima MC, Grantham-McGregor SM, Ashworth A. Development and behaviour of low-birthweight term infants at 8 years in northeast Brazil: a longitudinal study. Acta Paediatr 2006; 95: 1249-1257. CrossRef | PubMed
33 Sabet F, Richter LM, Ramchandani PG, Stein A, Quigley MA, Norris SA. Low birthweight and subsequent emotional and behavioural outcomes in 12-year-old children in Soweto, South Africa: findings from Birth to Twenty. Int J Epidemiol 2009; 38: 944-954. CrossRef | PubMed
34 Wang WL, Sung YT, Sung FC, Lu TH, Kuo SC, Li CY. Low birth weight, prematurity, and paternal social status: impact on the basic competence test in Taiwanese adolescents. J Pediatr 2008; 153: 333-338. CrossRef | PubMed
35 Kramer MS, Aboud F, Mironova E, et al. Breastfeeding and child cognitive development: new evidence from a large randomized trial. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2008; 65: 578-584. CrossRef | PubMed
36 Kramer MS, Fombonne E, Igumnov S, et al. Effects of prolonged and exclusive breastfeeding on child behavior and maternal adjustment: evidence from a large, randomized trial. Pediatrics 2008; 121: e435-e440. CrossRef | PubMed
37 Victora CG, Barros FC, Horta BL, Lima RC. Breastfeeding and school achievement in Brazilian adolescents. Acta Paediatr 2005; 94: 1656-1660. CrossRef | PubMed
38 Unay B, Sarici SU, Ulas UH, Akin R, Alpay F, Gokcay E. Nutritional effects on auditory brainstem maturation in healthy term infants. Arch Dis Child Fetal Neonatal Ed 2004; 89: F177-F179. PubMed
39 Adu-Afarwuah S, Lartey A, Brown KH, Zlotkin S, Briend A, Dewey KG. Randomized comparison of 3 types of micronutrient supplements for home fortification of complementary foods in Ghana: effects on growth and motor development. Am J Clin Nutr 2007; 86: 412-420. PubMed
40 Chen CM, Wang YY, Chang SY. Effect of in-home fortification of complementary feeding on intellectual development of Chinese children. Biomed Environ Sci 2010; 23: 83-91. CrossRef | PubMed
41 Alderman H, Hoddinott J, Kinsey B. Long term consequences of early childhood malnutrition. Oxf Econ Pap 2006; 58: 450-474. PubMed
42 Carba DB, Tan VL, Adair LS. Early childhood length-for-age is associated with the work status of Filipino young adults. Econ Hum Biol 2009; 7: 7-17. CrossRef | PubMed
43 Walker SP, Chang SM, Powell CA, Simonoff E, Grantham-McGregor SM. Early childhood stunting is associated with poor psychological functioning in late adolescence and effects are reduced by psychosocial stimulation. J Nutr 2007; 137: 2464-2469. PubMed
44 Crookston BT, Penny ME, Alder SC, et al. Children who recover from early stunting and children who are not stunted demonstrate similar levels of cognition. J Nutr 2010; 140: 1996-2001. CrossRef | PubMed
45 Stein AD, Wang M, DiGirolamo A, et al. Nutritional supplementation in early childhood, schooling, and intellectual functioning in adulthood: a prospective study in Guatemala. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med 2008; 162: 612-618. CrossRef | PubMed
46 Hoddinott J, Maluccio JA, Behrman JR, Flores R, Martorell R. Effect of a nutrition intervention during early childhood on economic productivity in Guatemalan adults. Lancet 2008; 371: 411-416. Summary | Full Text | PDF(89KB) | CrossRef | PubMed
47 Kordas K, Siegel EH, Olney DK, et al. The effects of iron and/or zinc supplementation on maternal reports of sleep in infants from Nepal and Zanzibar. J Dev Behav Pediatr 2009; 30: 131-139. CrossRef | PubMed
48 Peirano PD, Algarin CR, Garrido MI, Lozoff B. Iron deficiency anemia in infancy is associated with altered temporal organization of sleep states in childhood. Pediatr Res 2007; 62: 715-719. CrossRef | PubMed
49 Lozoff B, Smith JB, Clark KM, Perales CG, Rivera F, Castillo M. Home intervention improves cognitive and social—emotional scores in iron-deficient anemic infants. Pediatrics 2010; 126: e884-e894. CrossRef | PubMed
50 Shafir T, Angulo-Barroso R, Calatroni A, Jimenez E, Lozoff B. Effects of iron deficiency in infancy on patterns of motor development over time. Hum Mov Sci 2006; 25: 821-838. CrossRef | PubMed
51 Lukowski AF, Koss M, Burden MJ, et al. Iron deficiency in infancy and neurocognitive functioning at 19 years: evidence of long-term deficits in executive function and recognition memory. Nutr Neurosci 2010; 13: 54-70. CrossRef | PubMed
52 Corapci F, Calatroni A, Kaciroti N, Jimenez E, Lozoff B. Longitudinal evaluation of externalizing and internalizing behavior problems following iron deficiency in infancy. J Pediatr Psychol 2010; 35: 296-305. CrossRef | PubMed
53 Black MM, Baqui AH, Zaman K, et al. Iron and zinc supplementation promote motor development and exploratory behavior among Bangladeshi infants. Am J Clin Nutr 2004; 80: 903-910. PubMed
54 Dhingra P, Menon VP, SazawalS, et al. Effect of fortification of milk with zinc and iron along with vitamins C, E, A and selenium on growth, iron status and devlopment in preschool children—a community based double-masked randomized trial. 2nd World congress of Pediatric Gastroenterology, Hepatology and Nutrition; Paris, France; July 3—7, 2004.
55 Lorntz B, Soares AM, Moore SR, et al. Early childhood diarrhea predicts impaired school performance. Pediatr Infect Dis J 2006; 25: 513-520. CrossRef | PubMed
56 Patrick PD, Oria RB, Madhavan V, et al. Limitations in verbal fluency following heavy burdens of early childhood diarrhea in Brazilian shantytown children. Child Neuropsychol 2005; 11: 233-244. CrossRef | PubMed
57 Checkley W, Buckley G, Gilman RH, et al. Multi-country analysis of the effects of diarrhoea on childhood stunting. Int J Epidemiol 2008; 37: 816-830. PubMed
58 Kihara M, Carter JA, Newton CRJC. The effect of Plasmodium falciparum on cognition: a systematic review. Trop Med Int Health 2006; 11: 386-397. CrossRef | PubMed
59 Bangirana P, Giordani B, John CC, Page C, Opoka RO, Boivin MJ. Immediate neuropsychological and behavioral benefits of computerized cognitive rehabilitation in Ugandan pediatric cerebral malaria survivors. J Dev Behav Pediatr 2009; 30: 310-318. CrossRef | PubMed
60 Thuilliez J. Malaria and primary education: a cross-country analysis on repetition and completion rates. Revue d'économie du développement 2009; 2: 127-157. PubMed
61 Fernando D, De Silva D, Carter R, Mendis KN, Wickremasinghe R. A randomized, double-blind, placebo-controlled, clinical trial of the impact of malaria prevention on the educational attainment of school children. Am J Trop Med Hyg 2006; 74: 386-393. PubMed
62 Clarke SE, Jukes MC, Njagi JK, et al. Effect of intermittent preventive treatment of malaria on health and education in schoolchildren: a cluster-randomised, double-blind, placebo-controlled trial. Lancet 2008; 372: 127-138. Summary | Full Text | PDF(249KB) | CrossRef | PubMed
63 Fernando D, Wickremasinghe R, Mendis KN, Wickremasinghe AR. Cognitive performance at school entry of children living in malaria-endemic areas of Sri Lanka. Trans R Soc Trop Med Hyg 2003; 97: 161-165. CrossRef | PubMed
64 Olney DK, Pollitt E, Kariger PK, et al. Young Zanzibari children with iron deficiency, iron deficiency anemia, stunting, or malaria have lower motor activity scores and spend less time in locomotion. J Nutr 2007; 137: 2756-2762. PubMed
65 Jukes MC, Pinder M, Grigorenko EL, et al. Long-term impact of malaria chemoprophylaxis on cognitive abilities and educational attainment: follow-up of a controlled trial. PLoS Clin Trials 2006; 1: e19. PubMed
66 Van Rie A, Harrington PR, Dow A, Robertson K. Neurologic and neurodevelopmental manifestations of pediatric HIV/AIDS: a global perspective. Eur J Paediatr Neurol 2007; 11: 1-9. CrossRef | PubMed
67 Sherr L, Mueller J, Varrall R. A systematic review of cognitive development and child human immunodeficiency virus infection. Psychol Health Med 2009; 14: 387-404. CrossRef | PubMed
68 Chiriboga CA, Fleishman S, Champion S, Gaye-Robinson L, Abrams EJ. Incidence and prevalence of HIV encephalopathy in children with HIV infection receiving highly active anti-retroviral therapy (HAART). J Pediatr 2005; 146: 402-407. CrossRef | PubMed
69 Lindsey JC, Malee KM, Brouwers P, Hughes MD. Neurodevelopmental functioning in HIV-infected infants and young children before and after the introduction of protease inhibitor-based highly active antiretroviral therapy. Pediatrics 2007; 119: e681-e693. CrossRef | PubMed
70 Smith L, Adnams C, Eley B. Neurological and neurocognitive function of HIV infected children commenced on antiretroviral therapy. S Afr J Child Health 2008; 2: 108-113. PubMed
71 Van Rie A, Dow A, Mupuala A, Stewart P. Neurodevelopmental trajectory of HIV-infected children accessing care in Kinshasa, Democratic Republic of Congo. J Acquir Immune Defic Syndr 2009; 52: 636-642. CrossRef | PubMed
72 Van Rie A, Mupuala A, Dow A. Impact of the HIV/AIDS epidemic on the neurodevelopment of preschool-aged children in Kinshasa, Democratic Republic of the Congo. Pediatrics 2008; 122: e123-e128. CrossRef | PubMed
73 Sanmaneechai O, Puthanakit T, Louthrenoo O, Sirisanthana V. Growth, developmental, and behavioral outcomes of HIV-affected preschool children in Thailand. J Med Assoc Thai 2005; 88: 1873-1879. PubMed
74 Gray GE, Van Niekerk R, Struthers H, et al. The effects of adult morbidity and mortality on household welfare and the well-being of children in Soweto. Vulnerable Children Youth Studies 2006; 1: 15-28. PubMed
75 Floyd S, Crampin AC, Glynn JR, et al. The social and economic impact of parental HIV on children in northern Malawi: retrospective population-based cohort study. AIDS Care 2007; 19: 781-790. CrossRef | PubMed
76 Zabina H, Kissin D, Pervysheva E, et al. Abandonment of infants by HIV-positive women in Russia and prevention measures. Reprod Health Matters 2009; 17: 162-170. CrossRef | PubMed
77 Cluver L, Gardner F. The mental health of children orphaned by AIDS: a review of international and southern African research. J Child Adolesc Mental Health 2007; 19: 1-17. PubMed
78 Boris N, Thurman T, Snider L, Spencer E, Brown L. Infants and young children living in youth-headed households in Rwanda: implications of emerging data. Infant Mental Health Journal 2006; 27: 584-602. PubMed
79 Lusk D, Mararu J, O'Gara C, et al. Community care for orphans and AIDS affected children. Kakamega: The Academy for Educational Development/Speak for the Child, 2003.
80 Jedrychowski W, Perera FP, Jankowski J, et al. Very low prenatal exposure to lead and mental development of children in infancy and early childhood: Krakow prospective cohort study. Neuroepidemiology 2009; 32: 270-278. CrossRef | PubMed
81 Wang SX, Wang ZH, Cheng XT, et al. Arsenic and fluoride exposure in drinking water: children's IQ and growth in Shanyin county, Shanxi province, China. Environ Health Perspect 2007; 115: 643-647. CrossRef | PubMed
82 Marques RC, Dorea JG, Bernardi JV, Bastos WR, Malm O. Prenatal and postnatal mercury exposure, breastfeeding and neurodevelopment during the first 5 years. Cogn Behav Neurol 2009; 22: 134-141. CrossRef | PubMed
83 Myers GJ, Thurston SW, Pearson AT, et al. Postnatal exposure to methyl mercury from fish consumption: a review and new data from the Seychelles Child Development Study. Neurotoxicology 2009; 30: 338-349. CrossRef | PubMed
84 Davidson PW, Jean SR, Myers GJ, et al. Association between prenatal exposure to methylmercury and visuospatial ability at 10·7 years in the Seychelles Child Development Study. Neurotoxicology 2008; 29: 453-459. CrossRef | PubMed
85 Handal AJ, Lozoff B, Breilh J, Harlow SD. Effect of community of residence on neurobehavioral development in infants and young children in a flower-growing region of Ecuador. Environ Health Perspect 2007; 115: 128-133. CrossRef | PubMed
86 Torres-Sanchez L, Schnaas L, Cebrian ME, et al. Prenatal dichlorodiphenyldichloroethylene (DDE) exposure and neurodevelopment: a follow-up from 12 to 30 months of age. Neurotoxicology 2009; 30: 1162-1165. CrossRef | PubMed
87 Tang D, Li TY, Liu JJ, et al. Effects of prenatal exposure to coal-burning pollutants on children's development in China. Environ Health Perspect 2008; 116: 674-679. CrossRef | PubMed
88 Edwards SC, Jedrychowski W, Butscher M, et al. Prenatal exposure to airborne polycyclic aromatic hydrocarbons and children's intelligence at 5 years of age in a prospective cohort study in Poland. Environ Health Perspect 2010; 118: 1326-1331. CrossRef | PubMed
89 Mirza I, Tareen A, Davidson LL, Rahman A. Community management of intellectual disabilities in Pakistan: a mixed methods study. J Intellect Disabil Res 2009; 53: 559-570. CrossRef | PubMed
90 Pal DK, Chaudhury G, Sengupta S, Das T. Social integration of children with epilepsy in rural India. Soc Sci Med 2002; 54: 1867-1874. CrossRef | PubMed
91 Russell PS, al John JK, Lakshmanan JL. Family intervention for intellectually disabled children: randomised controlled trial. Br J Psychiatry 1999; 174: 254-258. CrossRef | PubMed
92 McConachie H, Huq S, Munir S, Ferdous S, Zaman S, Khan NZ. A randomized controlled trial of alternative modes of service provision to young children with cerebral palsy in Bangladesh. J Pediatr 2000; 137: 769-776. CrossRef | PubMed
93 Saloojee G, Phohole M, Saloojee H, IJsselmuiden C. Unmet health, welfare and educational needs of disabled children in an impoverished South African peri-urban township. Child Care Health Dev 2007; 33: 230-235. CrossRef | PubMed
94 Jin X, Sun Y, Jiang F, Ma J, Morgan C, Shen X. “Care for Development” intervention in rural China: a prospective follow-up study. J Dev Behav Pediatr 2007; 28: 213-218. CrossRef | PubMed
95 Nair MK, Philip E, Jeyaseelan L, George B, Mathews S, Padma K. Effect of Child Development Centre model early stimulation among at risk babies—a randomized controlled trial. Indian Pediatr 2009; 46 (suppl): s20-s26. PubMed
96 Potterton J, Stewart A, Cooper P, Becker P. The effect of a basic home stimulation programme on the development of young children infected with HIV. Dev Med Child Neurol 2010; 52: 547-551. CrossRef | PubMed
97 Baker-Henningham H, Walker SP, Powell C, Gardner JM. Preventing behaviour problems through a universal intervention in Jamaican basic schools: a pilot study. West Indian Med J 2009; 58: 460-464. PubMed
98 Nahar B, Hamadani JD, Ahmed T, et al. Effects of psychosocial stimulation on growth and development of severely malnourished children in a nutrition unit in Bangladesh. Eur J Clin Nutr 2009; 63: 725-731. CrossRef | PubMed
99 Kagitcibasi C, Sunar D, Bekman S, Baydar N, Cemalcilar Z. Continuing effects of early enrichment in adult life: The Turkish Early Enrichment Project 22 years later. J Appl Dev Psychol 2009; 30: 764-779. PubMed
100 Walker SP, Chang SM, Powell CA, Simonoff E, Grantham-McGregor SM. Effects of psychosocial stimulation and dietary supplementation in early childhood on psychosocial functioning in late adolescence: follow-up of randomised controlled trial. BMJ 2006; 333: 472. CrossRef | PubMed
101 Wachs TD, Black MM, Engle PL. Maternal depression: a global threat to children's health, development, and behavior and to human rights. Child Development Perspectives 2009; 3: 51-59. PubMed
102 Rahman A, Malik A, Sikander S, Roberts C, Creed F. Cognitive behaviour therapy-based intervention by community health workers for mothers with depression and their infants in rural Pakistan: a cluster-randomised controlled trial. Lancet 2008; 372: 902-909. Summary | Full Text | PDF(146KB) | CrossRef | PubMed
103 Almqvist K, Broberg AG. Young children traumatized by organized violence together with their mothers—the critical effects of damaged internal representations. Attach Hum Dev 2003; 5: 367-380. CrossRef | PubMed
104 Thabet AA, Karim K, Vostanis P. Trauma exposure in pre-school children in a war zone. Br J Psychiatry 2006; 188: 154-158. CrossRef | PubMed
105 Kithakye M, Morris AS, Terranova AM, Myers SS. The Kenyan political conflict and children's adjustment. Child Dev 2010; 81: 1114-1128. CrossRef | PubMed
106 Lustig S. An ecological framework for the refugee experience: what is the impact on child development?. In: Evans GW, Wachs TD, eds. Chaos and its influence on children's development: an ecological perpective. Washington, DC: American Psychological Association, 2010: 239-252.
107 Qouta S, Punamaki RL, El Sarraj E. Child development and family mental health in war and military violence: the Palestinian experience. Int J Behav Dev 2008; 32: 310-321. PubMed
108 Sadeh A, Hen-Gal S, Tikotzky L. Young children's reactions to war-related stress: a survey and assessment of an innovative intervention. Pediatrics 2008; 121: 46-53. CrossRef | PubMed
109 Rutter M, Sonuga-Barke EJ, Beckett C, et al. Deprivation-specific psychological patterns: effects of institutional deprivation. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev 2010; 75: 1-252. CrossRef | PubMed
110 van IJzendoorn MH, Lujik M, Juffer F. IQ of children growing up in children's homes a meta-analysis on IQ delays in orphanages. Merrill-Palmer Quarterly 2008; 54: 341-366. PubMed
111 Mehta MA, Golembo NI, Nosarti C, et al. Amygdala, hippocampal and corpus callosum size following severe early institutional deprivation: the English and Romanian Adoptees study pilot. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2009; 50: 943-951. CrossRef | PubMed
112 Fries AB, Shirtcliff EA, Pollak SD. Neuroendocrine dysregulation following early social deprivation in children. Dev Psychobiol 2008; 50: 588-599. CrossRef | PubMed
113 Tottenham N, Hare TA, Quinn BT, et al. Prolonged institutional rearing is associated with atypically large amygdala volume and difficulties in emotion regulation. Dev Sci 2010; 13: 46-61. CrossRef | PubMed
114 Marshall PJ, Reeb BC, Fox NA, Nelson CA, Zeanah CH. Effects of early intervention on EEG power and coherence in previously institutionalized children in Romania. Dev Psychopathol 2008; 20: 861-880. PubMed
115 Nelson CA, Furtado EA, Fox NA, Zeanah CH. The deprived human brain. Am Sci 2009; 97: 222-229. CrossRef | PubMed
116 The St Petersburg—USA Orphanage Research Team. The effects of early social-emotional and relationship experience on the development of young orphanage children. Monogr Soc Res Child Dev 2008; 73: 1-297. CrossRef | PubMed
117 van IJzendoorn MH, Juffer F. The Emanuel Miller Memorial Lecture 2006—adoption as intervention: meta-analytic evidence for massive catch-up and plasticity in physical, socio-emotional, and cognitive development. J Child Psychol Psychiatry 2006; 47: 1228-1245. CrossRef | PubMed
118 Barros AJ, Matijasevich A, Santos IS, Halpern R. Child development in a birth cohort: effect of child stimulation is stronger in less educated mothers. Int J Epidemiol 2010; 39: 285-294. CrossRef | PubMed
119 Castro DC, Lubker BB, Bryant DM, Skinner M. Oral language and reading abilities of first-grade Peruvian children: associations with child and family factors. Int J Behav Dev 2002; 26: 334-344. PubMed
120 Paxson C, Schady N. Cognitive development among young children in Ecuador: the roles of wealth, health, and parenting. J Hum Resour 2007; 42: 49-84. PubMed
121 Wang LW, Wang ST, Huang CC. Preterm infants of educated mothers have better outcome. Acta Paediatr 2008; 97: 568-573. CrossRef | PubMed
122 Stith AY, Gorman KS, Choudhury N. The effects of psychosocial risk and gender on school attainment in Guatemala. Applied Psychology 2003; 52: 614-629. PubMed
123 Boyle MH, Racine Y, Georgiades K, et al. The influence of economic development level, household wealth and maternal education on child health in the developing world. Soc Sci Med 2006; 63: 2242-2254. CrossRef | PubMed
124 Shin JY, Nhan NV, Lee SB, Crittenden KS, Flory M, Hong HT. The effects of a home-based intervention for young children with intellectual disabilities in Vietnam. J Intellect Disabil Res 2009; 53: 339-352. CrossRef | PubMed
125 Victora CG, Adair L, Fall C, et al. Maternal and child undernutrition: consequences for adult health and human capital. Lancet 2008; 371: 340-357. Summary | Full Text | PDF(317KB) | CrossRef | PubMed
126 Black MM, Walker SP, Wachs TD, et al. Policies to reduce undernutrition include child development. Lancet 2008; 371: 454-455. Full Text | PDF(44KB) | CrossRef | PubMed
127 Nores M, Barnett WS. Benefits of early childhood interventions across the world: (under) investing in the very young. Economics of Education Review 2009; 29: 271-282. PubMed
a Tropical Medicine Research Institute, The University of the West Indies, Kingston, Jamaica
b Department of Psychological Sciences, Purdue University, West Lafayette, IN, USA
c Institute of Child Health, London, UK
d Department of Pediatrics, University of Maryland, College Park, MD, USA
e Children's Hospital Boston/Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, USA
f Department of Nutrition, University of California, Davis, CA, USA
g Child Development Unit, ICDDR,B, Dhaka, Bangladesh
h Center for Human Growth and Development, Department of Pediatrics, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI, USA
i Caribbean Child Development Centre, The University of the West Indies, Open Campus, Jamaica
j Institute of Psychology, Health, and Society, University of Liverpool, Liverpool, UK
k Human Sciences Research Council & University of the Witwatersrand, South Africa
Corresponding
                          Author Information Corresponence to: Prof Susan P Walker, Tropical Medicine Research Institute, Epidemiology Research Unit, The University of the West Indies, Mona, Kingston, 7, Jamaica
Access this article on SciVerse ScienceDirect
Visit SciVerse ScienceDirect to see if you have access via your institution.
Article Options
Summary
Full Text
PDF (264 KB)
Printer Friendly Version
Download images
Request permission
Export Citation
Create Citation Alert
Receive an email when the article is cited
Bookmark